Personality creativity in language and communication
https://doi.org/10.28995/2686-7249-2022-8-182-192
Abstract
The article deals with the problem of personality creativity within the framework of the dichotomy “language VS communication”, which is built into the triad “language – creativity – cognition”. This triad is analyzed in two dimensions: from the point of view of the creativity of the language system; from the point of view of the creativity of a linguistic personality. Models and cognitive mechanisms of creativity are presented. Along with analogy and inference, there are three types of the main mechanism of creativity – deviations: pragmatic (distortion of the sign-user relationship); semantic (distortion of the sign-referent relationship); conceptual (distortion of the relationship between the conceptual structures of the original and new values). Pragmatic deviation plays a special role in the process of creative development of new meanings. The question of the boundaries of deviation and what imposes restrictions on the development of new creative meanings is considered. We prove that the limitations on the development of new creative meanings of the word are imposed by the conceptual structure of the original meaning of the word. Based on the latest achievements of Russian and foreign cognitive science, we analyze the structure of the mental lexicon as the main component that plays a leading role in creativity. The connection between creativity and the threat of manipulation is indicated. It is proved that linguistic creativity can lead to the creation of ambiguity (obscurity), which can lead to manipulation. The prospects for further research in the field of creativity are determined, namely, a cross-cultural approach to this phenomenon.
About the Author
V. I. ZabotkinaRussian Federation
Vera I. Zabotkina, Dr. of Sci. (Philology), professor
bld. 6, Miusskaya Sq., Moscow, 125047
References
1. Beaty, R.E. and Silvia, P.J. (2012), “Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks”, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, no. 6, pp. 309–319.
2. Beaty, R.E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P.J. and Schacter, D.L. (2016), “Creative cognition and brain network dynamics”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, no. 20, pp. 87–95.
3. Beaty, R.E., Silvia, P.J., Nusbaum, E.C., Jauk, E. and Benedek, M. (2014), “The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition”, Memory & Cognition, no. 42, pp. 1186–1197.
4. Benede, M., Franz, F., Heene, M. and Neubauer, A.C. (2012), “Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity”, Personality and Individual Differences, no. 53, pp. 480–485.
5. Boyarskaya, E.L. (1999), Kognitivnye osnovy formirovaniya novykh znachenii polisemanticheskikh sushchestvitel’nykh sovremennogo angliiskogo yazyka [Cognitive reasons of formation of new meanings with polysemantic nouns in modern English], Moscow, Russia.
6. Chomsky, N. (1957), Syntactic Structures, Mouton, The Hague, the Netherlands. Dem’yankov, V.Z. (2016), “Linguistic creativity in fiction” // Trudy instituta russkogo yazyka imeni V.V. Vinogradova, no. 7, pp. 29–45.
7. Feshchenko, V.V. (2008), Semiotika tvorchestva i lingvistika kreativnosti [Semiotics of creation and linguistics of creativity], Moscow, Russia.
8. Glaveanu, V.P. and Kaufman, J.C. (2019), “Creativity: a historical perspective”, in Kaufman, J.C. and Sternberg, R. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity, Cambridge, MA, USA.
9. Gruszka, A. and Necka, E. (2002), “Priming and acceptance of close and remote associations by creative and less creative people”, Creativity Research Journal, no. 14, pp. 193–205.
10. Iriskhanova, O.K. (2004), O lingvokreativnoi deyatel’nosti cheloveka. Otglagol’nye imena [Towards linguocreative activity of linguistic personality. Deverbal names], Izdatel’stvo VTII, Moscow, Russia.
11. Jauk, E., Benedek, M. and Neubauer, A.C. (2014), “The road to creative achievement: a latent variable model of ability and personality predictors”, European Journal of Personality, no. 28, pp. 95–105.
12. Jones, R.H. (2012), Discourse and creativity, Pearson Education, London, UK.
13. Kenett, Yo.N., Anaki, D. and Faust, M. (2014), “Investigating the structure of semantic networks in low and high creative persons”, Frontier in Human Neuroscience, no. 8, article 407, pp. 1–16.
14. Kenett, Yo.N., Beaty, R.E., Silvia, P.J., Anaki, D. and Faust, M. (2016), “Structure and Flexibility: Investigating the Relation Between the Structure of the Mental Lexicon, Fluid Intelligence, and Creative Achievement. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts”, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 377–388.
15. Kubryakova, E.S. (1987), Razmyshleniya ob analogii. Sushchnost’, razvitie i funktsii yazyka [Discussion on analogy. Essence, development, and functions of a language], Nauka, Moscow, Russia.
16. Kubryakova, E.S. (2009), Osnovnye napravleniya kontseptual’nogo analiza: vmesto vvedeniya [The main trends in conceptual analysis: instead of introduction], Institut yazykoznaniya RAN, Moscow, Russia.
17. Mednick, S.A. (1962), “The associative basis of the creative process”, Psychological Review, no. 69, pp. 220–232.
18. Paul, H. (1937), Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte, Halle a. S. Max Niemeyer, Halle (Saale), Tübingen, Germany, pp. 93–94.
19. Reiter-Palmon, R. and Leone, S. (2019), “Group dynamics and team creativity: the contribution of Kurt Lewin to the study of teams, creativity, and innovation”, in The creativity reader, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
20. Rossmann, E. and Fink, A. (2010), “Do creative people use shorter associative pathways?”, Personality and Individual Differences, vol. 49, pp. 891–895.
21. Swann, J. and Deumert, A. (2018), “Sociolinguistics and language creativity”, Language Sciences, vol. 65, pp. 1–8.
22. Swann, J., Pope, R. and Carter, R. (2011), Creativity in language and literature: the state of the art, Palgrave Macmillan, London, UK.
23. Tannen, D. (2007), Talking voices: repetition, dialogue and imagery in conversational discourse, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
24. Thagard, P. and Terrence, C.S. (2010), “The AHA! Experience: creativity through emergent binding in neural networks”, Cognitive science, no. 35, pp. 1–33.
25. Traugott, E.C. and Dasher, R. (2002), Regularity in semantic change, Cambridge, UK.
26. Yakobson, R. (1975), Lingvistika i poehtika. Strukturalizm “za” i “protiv” [Linguistics and poetics. Structuralism “for” and “against”], Progress, Moscow, Russia.
27. Zabotkina, V.I. (2014), .Slovo i smysl [Word and sense], RGGU, Moscow, Russia.
28. Zabotkina, V.I. (2017), “Neogelization of mental lexicon: cognitive and communicative parameters”, in Dem’yankov, V.Z. (ed.), Kognitsiya i kommunikatsiya v lingvisticheskikh issledovaniyakh: sbornik nauchnykh trudov [Cognition and communication in linguistic studies: collection of research papers], Izdatel’skii dom TGU imeni G.R. Derzhavina, Tambov, Russia, pp. 71–79. (Kognitivnye issledovaniya yazyka, vyp. 29)
29. Zabotkina, V.I. (2018), “Creativity of lexicon: cognition and communication interface”, in Zabotkina, V.I., Yazyk. Kul’tura. Perevod. Kommunikatsiya: sbornik nauchnykh trudov [Language. Culture. Translation. Communication: a collection of scientific works], issue 2, KDU, Universitetskaya kniga, Moscow, Russia, pp. 22–25.
30. Zykova, I.V and Kiose, M.I. (2020), “Parametrization of linguistic creativity in intercultural aspect: cinema discourse vs children’s literature discourse”, Voprosy kognitivnoi lingvistiki, issue 2, pp. 26–40.
Review
For citations:
Zabotkina V.I. Personality creativity in language and communication. RSUH/RGGU Bulletin: “Literary Teory. Linguistics. Cultural Studies”, Series. 2022;1(8-2):182-192. (In Russ.) https://doi.org/10.28995/2686-7249-2022-8-182-192