CONCEPTUAL ANALYSIS
Ways of presenting opinions depend on mental cultures which include i.a. styles of forming judgments on possibility and on probability of events and of states of affairs. Research on a large multilingual corpus of texts in several West-European languages and in Russian shows that the possibility statements are used more than twice as often as the probability statements.
The term ‘possibility’ in Latin and in modern languages denotes a physicalist attitude towards states of affairs. This term was coined much later than the term ‘probability’, originally connected to the human aspects of evaluation. The term ‘probabilis’ itself in Latin was a cognate of ‘probare’, which meant ‘approving’ and/or ‘controlling’ events. Additionally, in modern Romance languages, judgments of doubt and hope, i. e. sentences conveying speaker’s distancing from alien and non-actual opinions, usually contain verbs in a special ‘conjunctive mood’. Creating alternative ‘possible worlds’ as a figure of speech for ‘conjecture’ originated and was extensively used in writings by Leibniz in French, who used this figure in accordance with French grammar. The same ideas formulated in Russian or in German lack the ‘subjunctive’ mood, whereas it is obligatory in French.
PHILOSOPHY OF LANGUAGE AND COGNITIVE SCIENCES
The main meaning of the noun is presented in the article in the form of an ontoconcept – a hierarchy of three “age” concepts that are consistently formed in a child during his development. The process of formation of a specific ontoconcept CHAIR is considered in detail. At first (in 2–3 years), the child has the concept of HOLISTIC CHAIR, which represents the chair as an integral object and is used by the child in solving simple practical tasks related to the use of the chair for its intended purpose. Then a junior student develops the concept of SYSTEM CHAIR (a chair as a system of parts) from a holistic concept, which together with it forms a two-level ontoconcept of HOLISTIC CHAIR → SYSTEM CHAIR. Thanks to the system concept, a junior student can now solve more complex, technical tasks (repair a chair, assemble it from parts, etc.). Finally, a senior student forms FUNCTIONAL CHAIR (a system of functions of the referent’s parts) from a system concept, which joins a two-level ontoconcept and turns it into a three-level:
HOLISTIC CHAIR → SYSTEM CHAIR → FUNCTIONAL CHAIR.
Now the senior student can abstract from the appearance of the chair and use only its function: to form new seats by placing a stack of bricks against the wall and under, to see the seats in the bend of the tree trunk, in the step of the stairs, etc. Thus, this and other ontoconcepts serve as the basis for the realization of three relatively independent types of verbal thinking in humans: practical, technical and theoretical.